Gender effects of the COMT Val158Met genotype on verbal fluency in healthy adults
- Authors:
- Published online on: July 1, 2013 https://doi.org/10.3892/mmr.2013.1564
- Pages: 837-844
Abstract
Introduction
Gender differences in behavior and cognitive performance involve cultural and biological factors. Numerous parameters of brain function and structure differ between males and females (1–3); however, the exact mechanisms involved and how they affect each cognitive domain remain unclear. The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) enzyme has been implicated as a potential biological candidate involved in this gender dimorphism (4–6). To the best of our knowledge, there are a limited number of studies on this matter, the results of which are controversial.
Gender may impact cognitive function. Generally, females outperform males in their verbal abilities, while males outperform females in visuospatial tasks. Herlitz et al(7) investigated memory function and identified gender differences in the episodic memory that were in favor of females. Among individuals aged ≥85 years, females have demonstrated superior scores in cognitive speed and memory tasks, regardless of their often lower level of formal education (8). Halari et al(9) investigated whether sexually dimorphic cognitive performance in males and females was associated with sex hormones. Significant gender differences were observed, favoring males in the spatial and inhibition tasks, and favoring females in the verbal task (category fluency). However, no significant correlation was demonstrated between gender hormones and cognitive performance. van Hooren et al(10) analyzed the effects of age, education and gender on cognitive speed, verbal memory, executive function and verbal fluency in healthy older adults. A marked, age-related decrease in these tasks was identified. Education had a substantial effect on cognitive function; participants with a middle or high level of education demonstrated a superior performance in the cognitive tests (10). Additionally, females outperformed males in the verbal memory tasks (10).
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is an important region of the brain for cognition, and is strongly influenced by dopamine (DA). COMT is regarded as an important regulator of PFC DA levels, whereas the DA transporter (DAT) is not as widely found in the PFC as it is in the striatum (11,12). COMT is a key enzyme that is specifically involved in the metabolic degradation of extraneuronal DA (13). With regard to gender differences, one study identified a 17% increase in COMT enzyme activity in healthy males compared with that in healthy females (11). However, certain studies have demonstrated similar COMT levels and expression in both genders (11,14,15), while others have revealed higher levels in females (16). Males and females also appear to differentially regulate the abundance of novel mRNA (17) or protein (18) isoforms of COMT, which may alter the enzyme activity without affecting the total level of COMT transcripts or proteins (4). These results are concordant with earlier findings demonstrating a 30% increase in the activity of the enzyme in males compared with that in females (19). A similar difference was described in studies using human erythrocytes (20–22); however, one study was not concordant (23).
Genetic studies have suggested that COMT enzyme activity may also vary considerably according to COMT single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). The COMT gene SNP, rs4680, also known as Val158Met, reduces the activity of the enzyme in Met carriers (Met+ individuals) (24). This, in turn, lowers the enzyme activity and presumably increases the PFC DA levels (24,25). COMT exerts a significant regulatory effect on cognitive function (26–29), which involves the well-established effects of PFC DA levels on working memory and executive function (11,30–32). It has been suggested that the COMT rs4680 SNP may exert a direct effect on cognition in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls (26–29). The COMT SNP rs4680 Met allele has been demonstrated to be correlated with higher PFC DA levels (24,25), and with superior performance in working memory, intelligence and executive function in numerous studies (11,28,33–36). However, a number of studies did not reveal similar cognitive results (37–39). Furthermore, the COMT Met allele has been demonstrated to modulate cognitive dysfunction across mood episodes of bipolar disorder (40).
Results of previous COMT studies specifically concerned with the effects of gender/genotype on cognition require further clarification. Only a limited number of studies have demonstrated a gender-by-genotype effect on cognition (4–6). Two studies addressing the association between gender/genotype and cognition have focused on children and elderly individuals (5,6). Barnett et al(5) performed a range of cognitive tests in >5000 children (age, 8–10 years), including tests for IQ, attention span and working memory. Subjects were genotyped for the COMT Val158Met SNP, and the Met allele was found to be associated with improved function in several domains selectively among males. O'Hara et al(6) evaluated 163 older healthy adults and revealed that only Val/Val males performed superiorly in a test of delayed verbal recall, while a worse performance was observed in younger individuals.
Insufficient (hypodopaminergic) and excessive (hyperdopaminergic) DA receptor 1 (D1) stimulation has been demonstrated to impair PFC function in animal studies, resulting in cognitive dysfunction (41–43); however, the extension of these findings to healthy subjects remains speculative. Therefore, PFC cognition is hypothesized to depend on a specific level of DA in order to achieve optimal cognitive function (31,32). Given the limited data available on whether gender effects are associated with the COMT genotype interaction with cognition, the aim of the present study was to investigate this potential association in a homogeneous sample of healthy young adults. Based on studies demonstrating lower COMT activity in female COMT Met carriers, we hypothesized that females carrying the Met allele would have a lower cognitive performance due to altered PFC DA regulation.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Seventy-six healthy individuals (37 females and 39 males), aged 18–35 years (mean age, 23.2±3.26 years) with a mean schooling duration of 14.1±2.32 years and a mean IQ of 115.49±12.25, were recruited at the University of São Paulo (São Paulo, Brazil). Study participants were predominantly medical students. None of the subjects had a past history of or were currently suffering with a psychiatric disorder, according to the evaluation conducted by psychiatrists using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (44). In addition, all subjects had no family history (amongst first-degree relatives) of mood or psychotic disorders, and had not used psychotropic substances or indulged in substance abuse within the last three months. Only females with a regular menstrual cycle, taking oral contraceptives, were included.
Neurocognitive assessments
A range of neurocognitive assessments were designed to assess the following domains: i) Attention, by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III [WAIS-III, including the Digit Span (DS) subtest] and the Trail Making Test, part A (TMT-A); ii) memory, using the immediate and delayed Logical Memory subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-LM1 and -LM2, respectively); iii) verbal fluency, by the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (FAS); and iv) executive function, assessed by the TMT-B (45–48). Experienced clinical neuropsychologists performed the neurocognitive assessments. Raw scores, corrected for demographic factors, were used given the absence of standardized scores for the Brazilian population.
Genotyping
DNA was extracted from the peripheral blood according to the salting out procedure (49), and was then genotyped for COMT rs4680 using quantitative (q)PCR allelic discrimination. PCR amplification for rs4680 was performed in 5-μl reactions with 5 ng template DNA, 1X TaqMan Universal Master mix (Applied Biosystems, Inc., Foster City, CA, USA), each primer and probe assay, and H2O. Thermal cycling consisted of initial denaturation for 10 min at 95°C, followed by 40 cycles of denaturation at 95°C for 15 sec, and annealing at 60°C for 1 min. The allele detection process and allelic discrimination were performed for 1 min at 60°C on a 7500 Real-Time PCR system (Applied Biosystems, Inc.). Quality control of qPCR results was achieved by direct sequencing using an ABI Prism 3100 Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems, Inc.).
Statistical analyses
Cognitive tests were stratified as a function of COMT rs4680 genotype (Met/Met, Val/Met and Val/Val) and functional allele [Met+ (Met/Met or Met/Val) or Met− (Val/Val)]. The cognitive test results had a normal distribution, and parametric tests were used to analyze the data. The multivariate analysis of the covariance test was performed using the cognitive test results as factors, and age, gender, education, genotype and Met allele as covariates. COMT genotype and gender, as well as COMT Met allele and gender, interactions were also analyzed. P<0.05 was considered to indicate a statistically significant difference. All statistical analyses were performed using the PASW statistical software, version 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Correction for multiple comparisons was performed using the Bonferroni test.
Ethics
The research ethics board of the Clinics Hospital of the University of São Paulo approved this study. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.
Results
No statistically significant differences in the sociodemographic data (age, gender and educational level) were observed among the COMT genotypes (Table I). Moreover, there were no differences in gender, age, education or IQ (Table I). The COMT genotype distributions in the experimental samples of males and females were identified to be in accordance with the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (χ2=0.65), thus indicating representative samples. The allelic frequency of rs4680 was 51.3% for the Met allele and 48.7% for the Val allele.
A multivariate general linear test model, using age, educational level, gender, COMT rs4680 genotype and the interaction between COMT genotype and gender as covariates, was implemented.
Age negatively influenced cognitive performance on the WAIS-DS-FW (P=0.02, partial η2, 7.66%), TMT-A (P<0.001, partial η2, 17.4%) and TMT-B (P=0.02, partial η2, 7.57%) (Table II). Gender influenced WAIS-DS (P=0.01, partial η2, 9.1%) (Table II). Females performed better than males in both tests. The COMT genotype had no influence on cognitive performance after Bonferroni correction. Gender and COMT genotype interaction influenced the FAS total score (P=0.03, partial η2, 6.67%) and FAS letter S (P=0.01, partial η2, 8.0%) (Table III). Among males, Met homozygotes had the highest scores, Met heterozygotes had intermediate performance while Val homozygotes had the lowest scores. Among females, Val homozygotes had the highest scores, while Met homozygotes had the lowest scores (Fig 1).
Table IIMultivariate analysis of cognitive tests, with age, education, gender, COMT allele Met and gender*COMT allele Met interaction as the cofactors. |
Table IIIMultivariate analysis of cognitive tests, with age, education, gender, COMT rs4680 genotype and gender*COMT rs4680 genotype interaction as the cofactors. |
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the effect of the interaction between gender and the COMT rs4680 Met allele on verbal fluency, with opposite results identified for each gender. Female carriers of the Met allele had a lower performance than males in a verbal fluency test. In addition, subjects that were heterozygous (Val/Met) for COMT rs4680 had an intermediate performance in the verbal fluency test.
The results suggested a specific gender-dependent effect of Met+ on verbal fluency, reinforcing the hypothesis that there is a distinct optimal DA activity/level for different components of cognition (28,50). Furthermore, the COMT Met allele negatively influenced cognitive performance (verbal fluency) in females compared with that in males (Fig. 1). Given that females have a lower COMT activity than males, per se(11,19), one possible explanation of our findings is that females carrying the low activity Met allele are likely to exceed the optimal PFC DA levels, thus receiving no benefit from its excessively high levels.
The effects of DA on neurocognition have been previously described. Several studies have demonstrated that lower doses of D1 agonists enhance working memory and attention span (31,51), while higher levels of DA impair PFC function (52). Low doses of psychostimulants in hyperkinetic children have been demonstrated to be associated with significant improvements in short-term memory, whereas higher doses worsen cognitive performance (53). Similar dose-dependent effects were also observed in healthy volunteers using dextroamphetamine, a drug that potentiates dopaminergic activity (52). Studies on COMT functional SNPs and the differential effects of D1 and D2 receptor binding have also demonstrated cognitive decline to be associated with COMT activity. Healthy subjects carrying the Met+ allele (rs4680) exhibited lower phasic and higher subcortical tonic DA transmission, which was proposed to be associated with an increase in central DA levels (28,54).
COMT enzyme activity appears to have a gender-specific effect; molecular postmortem studies have revealed that COMT enzyme activity in the PFC was 17% higher in males compared with that in females (11). These results are concordant with earlier findings demonstrating a 30% higher enzyme activity in males compared with females (19). A similar difference was described in studies using human erythrocytes (20–22); however, one study did not find differences between genders (23). Notably, 17-b-estradiol (E2) administration decreased COMT activity in the rat liver (55,56). Similarly, Xie et al(1999) demonstrated that there are two estrogen response elements in the COMT promoter, and that, at physiological concentrations, E2 inhibited COMT mRNA expression in cells expressing estrogen receptors, but not in cells that did not express these receptors. An estrogen-mediated decrease in COMT mRNA was also accompanied by a proportional decrease in COMT immunoreactivity and activity (55,56). In another study, the PFC DA levels were affected in male, but not female, COMT knockout mice (13). One possible explanation for this gender-distinct function is the bidirectional association between COMT and estrogen-related compounds, whereby COMT activity varies inversely according to estrogen levels (29). Moreover, estrogen levels affect the striatal dopaminergic system (57,58), and affect cognition (41–43,59,60). Overall, these findings may aid in the clarification of the role of gender-specific effects on COMT-modulated cognition.
Studies investigating the cognitive effects of DA in the PFC have mainly focused on the D1 receptor, the predominant type of receptor in the PFC. Hypodopaminergic and hyperdopaminergic D1 receptor stimulation have been demonstrated to impair PFC function (31,32,41–43), resulting in cognitive dysfunction. Therefore, PFC cognition may depend on an optimal level of DA to achieve normal function (31,32,51,52). These kinetics have been described by an inverted-U response function model in pharmacological studies (31,51,52). In this model, the effect of amphetamine and other drugs on cognition is described as an inverted-U shape, in which the peak is the threshold for maximum cognitive performance, with subsequent decline thereafter. We propose that the inverted-U shape model, which states that an optimal level of DA is required to benefit cognitive function, represents a plausible explanation as to why female Met allele carriers exhibit a worse performance in verbal fluency tests. The basis for this explanation is that females have lower COMT activity per se, which may be due to the effect of estrogen on this enzyme (29). Consequently, at least theoretically, male and female Met+ subjects have different baseline DA levels.
The FAS is also considered to be a test of executive functions, including cognitive organization, initiation, maintenance of effort and the ability to conduct a non-routine search for words based on a specific first letter, rather than the lexical definition (61–63). This interpretation is consistent with studies demonstrating a worse performance in individuals with frontal lobe lesions (63,64), and sensitivity to cognitive dysfunction in disorders that affect executive functions (65). Effects of demographic variables are important to consider when interpreting FAS results. Age effects have not been identified in numerous studies (64,66,67); however, a number of studies have demonstrated modest age effects, with higher age predicting a worse performance (68). A higher level of education has been associated with improved FAS performance in several studies (64,67,69). In the present study, neither age nor education influenced FAS. However, with regard to gender, a superior FAS performance was observed in females. Previous studies have demonstrated controversial results; a number of studies identified superior FAS performance in females compared with males (66,70), while other studies did not observe a difference between the two genders (69,71).
Limitations of this study included the absence of information with regard to the menstrual cycle of participants. Furthermore, the use of oral contraceptives may have affected COMT activity.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to identify a gender-specific effect of COMT SNP rs4680 on verbal fluency in young healthy subjects, with opposite effects on performance in each gender. The presence of the Met allele in female subjects was associated with worse verbal fluency; while in males, it was correlated with an improvement in this particular cognitive function. Our results suggested that DA activity affects cognitive function in different ways, according to gender, most likely due to COMT gender differences.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank the Institute of Psychiatry at the University of São Paulo, particularly the members of the Mood Disorders Unit (GRUDA) and the Laboratory of Neuroscience (LIM27), for their dedication and hard work, as well as the volunteers, for their collaboration. The São Paulo Research Foundation [Fundo de Apoio a Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP), 2010/06230-0)] and the Association Beneficente Alzira Denise Hertzog Da Silva (ABADHS), provided financial support for this study.
References
Murphy DG, DeCarli C, McIntosh AR, Daly E, Mentis MJ, Pietrini P, et al: Sex differences in human brain morphometry and metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography study on the effect of aging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 53:585–594. 1996. View Article : Google Scholar | |
Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Horton NJ, Makris N, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS Jr, et al: Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human brain assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex. 11:490–497. 2001. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
De Vries GJ: Minireview: Sex differences in adult and developing brains: compensation, compensation, compensation. Endocrinology. 145:1063–1068. 2004.PubMed/NCBI | |
Harrison PJ and Tunbridge EM: Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): a gene contributing to sex differences in brain function, and to sexual dimorphism in the predisposition to psychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 33:3037–3045. 2008. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Barnett JH, Heron J, Ring SM, Golding J, Goldman D, Xu K and Jones PB: Gender-specific effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val108/158Met polymorphism on cognitive function in children. Am J Psychiatry. 164:142–149. 2007.PubMed/NCBI | |
O'Hara R, Miller E, Liao CP, Way N, Lin X and Hallmayer J: COMT genotype, gender and cognition in community-dwelling, older adults. Neurosci Lett. 409:205–209. 2006. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Herlitz A, Nilsson LG and Bäckman L: Gender differences in episodic memory. Mem Cognit. 25:801–811. 1997. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
van Exel E, Gussekloo J, de Craen AJ, Bootsma-van der Wiel A, Houx P, Knook DL and Westendorp RG: Cognitive function in the oldest old: women perform better than men. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 71:29–32. 2001. | |
Halari R, Hines M, Kumari V, Mehrotra R, Wheeler M, Ng V and Sharma T: Sex differences and individual differences in cognitive performance and their relationship to endogenous gonadal hormones and gonadotropins. Behav Neurosci. 119:104–117. 2005. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
van Hooren SA, Valentijn AM, Bosma H, Ponds RW, van Boxtel MP and Jolles J: Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: a cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 14:40–54. 2007. | |
Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, et al: Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet. 75:807–821. 2004. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Wayment HK, Schenk JO and Sorg BA: Characterization of extracellular dopamine clearance in the medial prefrontal cortex: role of monoamine uptake and monoamine oxidase inhibition. J Neurosci. 21:35–44. 2001.PubMed/NCBI | |
Gogos JA, Morgan M, Luine V, Santha M, Ogawa S, Pfaff D and Karayioegou M: Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 95:9991–9996. 1998. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Bray NJ, Buckland PR, Williams NM, Williams HJ, Norton N, Owen MJ and O'Donovan MC: A haplotype implicated in schizophrenia susceptibility is associated with reduced COMT expression in human brain. Am J Hum Genet. 73:152–161. 2003. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Tunbridge E, Burnet PW, Sodhi MS and Harrison PJ: Catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) and proline dehydrogenase (PRODH) mRNAs in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression. Synapse. 51:112–118. 2004. View Article : Google Scholar | |
Dempster EL, Mill J, Craig IW and Collier DA: The quantification of COMT mRNA in post mortem cerebellum tissue: diagnosis, genotype, methylation and expression. BMC Med Genet. 7:102006. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Tunbridge EM, Lane TA and Harrison PJ: Expression of multiple catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) mRNA variants in human brain. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 144B:834–839. 2007. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Tunbridge EM, Harrison PJ and Weinberger DR: Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond. Biol Psychiatry. 60:141–151. 2006. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Boudíková B, Szumlanski C, Maidak B and Weinshilboum R: Human liver catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 48:381–389. 1990. | |
Fähndrich E, Coper H, Christ W, Helmchen H, Müller-Oerlinghausen B and Pietzcker A: Erythrocyte COMT-activity in patients with affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 61:427–437. 1980. | |
Floderus Y and Wetterberg L: The inheritance of human erythrocyte catechol-O-methyltransferase activity. Clin Genet. 19:392–395. 1981. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Philippu G, Hoo JJ, Milech U, Argarwall DP, Schrappe O and Goedde HW: Catechol-O-methyltransferase of erythrocytes in patients with endogenous psychoses. Psychiatry Res. 4:139–146. 1981. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Fitzgerald GA, Hamilton CA, Jones DH and Reid JL: Erythrocytes catechol-O-methyltransferase activity and indices of sympathetic activity in man. Clin Sci (Lond). 58:423–425. 1980.PubMed/NCBI | |
Lachman HM, Papolos DF, Saito T, Yu YM, Szumlanski CL and Weinshilboum RM: Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics. 6:243–250. 1996. View Article : Google Scholar | |
Weinshilboum RM, Otterness DM and Szumlanski CL: Methylation pharmacogenetics: catechol O-methyltransferase, thiopurine methyltransferase, and histamine N-methyltransferase. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 39:19–52. 1999. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE, et al: Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 98:6917–6922. 2001. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Joober R, Gauthier J, Lal S, Bloom D, Lalonde P, Rouleau G, et al: Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val-108/158-Met gene variants associated with performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 59:662–663. 2002. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Bilder RM, Volavka J, Lachman HM and Grace AA: The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 29:1943–1961. 2004. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Diamond A, Briand L, Fossella J and Gehlbach L: Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. Am J Psychiatry. 161:125–132. 2004. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Meyer-Lindenberg A, Nichols T, Callicott JH, Ding J, Kolachana B, Buckholtz J, et al: Impact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function. Mol Psychiatry. 11:867–877. 7972006. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Mehta MA, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Mavaddat N, Pickard JD and Robbins TW: Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. J Neurosci. 20:RC652000.PubMed/NCBI | |
Goldman-Rakic PS, Castner SA, Svensson TH, Siever LJ and Williams GV: Targeting the dopamine D1 receptor in schizophrenia: insights for cognitive dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 174:3–16. 2004.PubMed/NCBI | |
Caldú X, Vendrell P, Bartrés-Faz D, Clemente I, Bargalló N, Jurado MA, et al: Impact of the COMT Val108/158 Met and DAT genotypes on prefrontal function in healthy subjects. Neuroimage. 37:1437–1444. 2007.PubMed/NCBI | |
Bruder GE, Keilp JG, Xu H, Shikhman M, Schori E, Gorman JM and Gilliam TC: Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genotypes and working memory: associations with differing cognitive operations. Biol Psychiatry. 58:901–907. 2005. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
de Frias CM, Annerbrink K, Westberg L, Eriksson E, Adolfsson R and Nilsson LG: COMT gene polymorphism is associated with declarative memory in adulthood and old age. Behav Genet. 34:533–539. 2004.PubMed/NCBI | |
de Frias CM, Annerbrink K, Westberg L, Eriksson E, Adolfsson R and Nilsson LG: Catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism is associated with cognitive performance in nondemented adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 17:1018–1025. 2005.PubMed/NCBI | |
Barnett JH, Scoriels L and Munafò MR: Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry. 64:137–144. 2008. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Dennis NA, Need AC, LaBar KS, Waters-Metenier S, Cirulli ET, Kragel J, et al: COMT val108/158 met genotype affects neural but not cognitive processing in healthy individuals. Cereb Cortex. 20:672–683. 2010. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Solís-Ortiz S, Pérez-Luque E, Morado-Crespo L and Gutiérrez-Muñoz M: Executive functions and selective attention are favored in middle-aged healthy women carriers of the Val/Val genotype of the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene: a behavioral genetic study. Behav Brain Funct. 6:672010.PubMed/NCBI | |
Soeiro de Souza MG, Machado-Vieira R, Soares Bio D, Do Prado CM and Moreno RA: COMT polymorphisms as predictors of cognitive dysfunction during manic and mixed episodes in bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disord. 14:554–564. 2012.PubMed/NCBI | |
Zahrt J, Taylor JR, Mathew RG and Arnsten AF: Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. J Neurosci. 17:8528–8535. 1997.PubMed/NCBI | |
Granon S, Passetti F, Thomas KL, Dalley JW, Everitt BJ and Robbins TW: Enhanced and impaired attentional performance after infusion of D1 dopaminergic receptor agents into rat prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 20:1208–1215. 2000.PubMed/NCBI | |
Arnsten AFT and Li BM: Neurobiology of executive functions: catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biol Psychiatry. 57:1377–1384. 2005. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al: The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 59(Suppl 20): 22–33; quiz 34–57. 1998.PubMed/NCBI | |
Strauss E, Sherman EMS and Spreen O: A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary. 3rd edition. Oxford University Press, Inc; New York, NY: 2006 | |
Wechsler D: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. The Psychological Corporation: Harcourt Brace and Company; New York, NY: 1999 | |
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1981 | |
Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press, Inc; New York, NY: 2004 | |
Laitinen J, Samarut J and Hölttä E: A nontoxic and versatile protein salting-out method for isolation of DNA. Biotechniques. 17:316318320–322. 1994.PubMed/NCBI | |
Clark L, Cools R and Robbins TW: The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: decision-making and reversal learning. Brain Cogn. 55:41–53. 2004. View Article : Google Scholar | |
Kimberg DY, D'Esposito M and Farah MJ: Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. Neuroreport. 8:3581–3585. 1997. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Mattay VS, Goldberg TE, Fera F, Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Egan F, et al: Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 100:6186–6191. 2003. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Sprague RL and Sleator EK: Methylphenidate in hyperkinetic children: differences in dose effects on learning and social behavior. Science. 198:1274–1276. 1977. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Cousins DA, Butts K and Young AH: The role of dopamine in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 11:787–806. 2009. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Jiang H, Xie T, Ramsden DB and Ho SL: Human catechol-O-methyltransferase down-regulation by estradiol. Neuropharmacology. 45:1011–1018. 2003. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Cohn CK and Axelrod J: The effect of estradiol on catechol-O-methyltransferase activity in rat liver. Life Sci I. 10:1351–1354. 1971. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Lindamer LA, Lohr JB, Harris MJ and Jeste DV: Gender, estrogen, and schizophrenia. Psychopharmacol Bull. 33:221–228. 1997.PubMed/NCBI | |
Halbreich U: Role of estrogen in postmenopausal depression. Neurology. 48(Suppl 7): S16–S19. 1997. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Colzato LS, Hertsig G, van den Wildenberg WP and Hommel B: Estrogen modulates inhibitory control in healthy human females: evidence from the stop-signal paradigm. Neuroscience. 167:709–715. 2010. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Gasbarri A, Pompili A, d'Onofrio A, Cifariello A, Tavares MC and Tomaz C: Working memory for emotional facial expressions: role of the estrogen in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 33:964–972. 2008. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Andrewes D: Neuropsychology From Theory to Practice. Psychology Press; New York, NY: 2001 | |
Devinsky O and D'Esposito M: Neurology of Cognitive and Behavioral Disorders. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 2004 | |
Darby D and Walsh KW: Walsh's Neuropsychology: A Clinical Approach. 5th edition. Elsevier Churchill Livingstone; Edinburgh: 2005 | |
Ruff RM, Light RH, Parker SB and Levin HS: Benton Controlled Oral Word Association Test: reliability and updated norms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 11:329–338. 1996. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Henry JD and Beatty WW: Verbal fluency deficits in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychologia. 44:1166–1174. 2006. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Bolla KI, Lindgren KN, Bonaccorsy C and Bleecker ML: Predictors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. J Clin Psychol. 46:623–628. 1990. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Selnes OA, Jacobson L, Machado AM, Becker JT, Wesch J, Miller EN, et al: Normative data for a brief neuropsychological screening battery. Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Percept Mot Skills. 73:539–550. 1991. View Article : Google Scholar : PubMed/NCBI | |
Libon D, David J, Glosser G, Malamut BL, Kaplan E, Goldberg E, et al: Age, executive functions, and visuospatial functioning in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology. 8:38–43. 1994. View Article : Google Scholar | |
Tombaugh TN, Kozak J and Rees L: Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 14:167–177. 1999.PubMed/NCBI | |
Ruff RM, Allen CC, Farrow CE, Niemann H and Wylie T: Figural fluency: differential impairment in patients with left versus right frontal lobe lesions. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 9:41–55. 1994. View Article : Google Scholar | |
Miller BL and Cummings JL: The Human Frontal Lobes: Functions and Disorders. 2nd edition. The Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2007 |